dc.rights.license | Atribución-NoComercial-SinDerivadas 4.0 Internacional |
dc.contributor.advisor | Clavijo Álvarez, Álvaro Arturo |
dc.contributor.advisor | Hurtado Parrado, Camilo |
dc.contributor.author | Henao Barbosa, Karen Viviana |
dc.date.accessioned | 2020-02-25T15:54:24Z |
dc.date.available | 2020-02-25T15:54:24Z |
dc.date.issued | 2020-02-16 |
dc.identifier.uri | https://repositorio.unal.edu.co/handle/unal/75727 |
dc.description.abstract | Exposure to non-contingent aversive stimulation, such as high intensity noise, decreases self-controlled behavior in humans. In addition, physiological responses in humans exposed to aversive visual stimuli are similar to those observed in other species. This study evaluated whether the disruptive effects of non-contingent aversive stimulation on self-control are replicated by aversive visual stimulation and what the response pattern looks like in alternative contexts. We also analyzed the values of delay sensitivity (k) and Area Under the Curve (AUC) of the participants in relation to self-control behavior. Non-contingent exposure to aversive visual stimulation only generated an impulsive choice pattern in the short term. The values of k and AUC did not indicate how all participants behaved. Although, the k-value has been shown to be a relatively long-lasting factor in people, it should be evaluated with caution if it can be an indicator of choice behavior in alternative situations. |
dc.description.abstract | La exposición a estimulación aversiva no contingente, como el ruido de alta intensidad, disminuye la conducta autocontrolada en humanos. Además, las respuestas fisiológicas en humanos expuestos a imágenes aversivas son similares a las que se observan en otras especies. Este estudio evaluó, si los efectos disruptivos de la estimulación aversiva no contingente sobre el auto-contro replican con estimulación visual aversiva y cómo es el patrón de respuesta en contextos alternativos. Se analizaron también los valores de sensibilidad a la demora (k) y Área Bajo la Curva (AUC) de los participantes en relación con el comportamiento de auto-control. La exposición no contingente a estimulación visual aversiva sólo generó un patrón de elección impulsiva a corto plazo. Los valores de k y AUC no indicaron cómo se comportaron todos los participantes. Aunque, el valor de k ha mostrado ser un factor relativamente duradero en las personas, debe evaluarse con precaución sí puede ser indicador del comportamiento de elección en situaciones alternativas. |
dc.format.extent | 66 |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf |
dc.language.iso | spa |
dc.rights | Derechos reservados - Universidad Nacional de Colombia |
dc.rights.uri | http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ |
dc.subject.ddc | Psicología |
dc.title | Efectos de la estimulación visual aversiva no contingente sobre la conducta de elección |
dc.type | Otro |
dc.rights.spa | Acceso abierto |
dc.description.additional | Magíster en Psicología |
dc.type.driver | info:eu-repo/semantics/other |
dc.type.version | info:eu-repo/semantics/acceptedVersion |
dc.description.degreelevel | Maestría |
dc.publisher.department | Departamento de Psicología |
dc.publisher.branch | Universidad Nacional de Colombia - Sede Bogotá |
dc.relation.references | American Psychological Association (2010) Principios Éticos de los Psicólogos y Código de Conducta. Recuperado de: http://www.psicologia.unam.mx/documentos/pdf/comite_etica/Codigo_APA.pdf |
dc.relation.references | Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious Reward: A Behavioral Theory of Impulsiveness and Impulse Control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 4. |
dc.relation.references | Azevedo, T. M., Volchan, E., Imbiriba, L. A., Rodrigues, E. C., Oliveira, J. M., Oliveira, L… & Vargas, C. D. (2005). A freezing-like posture to pictures of mutilation. Psychophysiology, 42, 255–260. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00287.x |
dc.relation.references | Białaszeka, W., & Ostaszewski, P. (2012). Discounting of sequences of delayed rewards of different amounts. Behavioural Processes, 89, 39– 43. |
dc.relation.references | Bickel, W. K., Odum, A. L., & Madden, G. J. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology, 146, 447–454 |
dc.relation.references | Bradley, M., & Lang, P.J. (2007). The International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in the study of emotion and attention. In J.A. Coan and J. J. B. Allen (Eds.). Handbook of emotion elicitation and assessment (pp. 29 - 46). New York: Oxford University Press. |
dc.relation.references | Brunette, M., & Jacob J. (2019). Risk aversion, prudence and temperance: An experiment in gain and loss. Research in Economics, 73, 174–189. DOI: 10.1016/j.rie.2019.04.004 |
dc.relation.references | Crosbie, J. (1998). Negative reinforcement and punishment. In K. Lattal & M. Perone (Eds.), Handbook of Research Methods in Human Operant Behavior (pp. 163–187). New York: Plenum Press. |
dc.relation.references | Du, W., Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2002). Cross-cultural comparisons of discounting delayed and probabilistic rewards. The Psychological Record, 52, 479-492 |
dc.relation.references | Facchinetti, L. D., Imbiriba, L. A., Azevedo, T. M., Vargas, C. D., & Volchan, E. (2006). Postural modulation induced by pictures depicting prosocial or dangerous contexts. Neuroscience Letters, 410, 52–56. |
dc.relation.references | Flora, S. R., Schieferecke, T. R., & Bremenkamp, H. G. (1992). Effects of aversive noise on human self-control for positive reinforcement. The Psychological Record, 42, 505-517. |
dc.relation.references | Flora, S. R., Wilkerson, L. R., & Flora, D. B. (2003). Effects of cold pressor pain on human self-control for positive reinforcement. The Psychological Record, 53, 243–252. |
dc.relation.references | Frye, C. C., Galizio, A., Friedel, J. E., DeHart, W. B., & Odum, A. L. (2016). Measuring Delay Discounting in Humans Using an Adjusting Amount Task. Journal of Visualized Experiments. (107), e53584. DOI:10.3791/53584. |
dc.relation.references | Gantiva, D. C., Guerra, M. P., & Vila, C. J. (2011). Validación Colombiana del Sistema Internacional de Imágenes Afectivas: Evidencias del origen transcultural de la emoción. Acta Colombiana de Psicología. 14, 103-111. |
dc.relation.references | Green, L., Fry, A. F., & Myerson, J. (1994). Discounting of Delayed Rewards: A Life-Span Comparison. Psychological Science, 5, 33. |
dc.relation.references | Green, L., Myerson, J., & Calvert, A. L. (2010). Pigeons’ discounting of probabilistic and delayed reinforcers. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 94, 113-123. |
dc.relation.references | Grossbard, C. L., & Mazur, J. E. (1986). A comparison of delays and ratio requirements in self-control choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 305-315. |
dc.relation.references | Hurtado-Parrado, C. Bohórquez, M. C., Sánchez, C., Cifuentes, J., Forigua, J. C., & Arias-Higuera, M. (2019). Aversive control of impulsive behavior: effects of noncontingent aversive visual stimulation. Paper presented during the 45th Annual Convention Association for Behavior Analysis (Chicago, USA, May 27, 2019) |
dc.relation.references | Hurtado-Parrado, C. Gomez, A., Cifuentes, J., Forigua, J. C., Velázquez, J. C., Amortegui, M., Tovar, D., Quintana, S., & Segura, A. (2019). Effects of non-contingent shocks on self-control behavior and preference reversal in Wistar rats. Poster presented during the Society for Quantitative Analysis of Behavior Annual Convention (Chicago, May 22, 2019). |
dc.relation.references | Hurtado-Parrado, C., González-León, C., Arias-Higuera, M, A., Cardona, A., Medina, L, G., García-Muñoz, L… & Rico, J, L. (2017). Assessing Mongolian gerbil emotional behavior: effects of two shock intensities and response-independent shocks during an extended inhibitory-avoidance task. PeerJ, 5:e4009; DOI 10.7717/peerj.4009. |
dc.relation.references | Jimura, K., Myerson, J., Hilgard, J., Braver, T, S & Green, L. (2009). Are people really more patient than other animals? Evidence from human discounting of real liquid rewards. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16 (6), 1071-1075. DOI:10.3758/PBR.16.6.1071. |
dc.relation.references | Johnson, P. S., Herrmann, E. S., & Johnson, M. W. (2015). Opportunity costs of reward delays and the discounting of hypothetical money and cigarettes. Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior, 103, 87–107. |
dc.relation.references | Logue, A. W. (1988). Research on self-control: An integrating framework. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 666-709. |
dc.relation.references | Logue, A. W., & King, G. R. (1991) Self-control and impulsiveness in adult humans when food is the reinforcer. Appetite, 17, 105-120. |
dc.relation.references | Mazur, J. E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M. L. Commons, J. E. Mazur, J. A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior, Vol. 5. The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value (p. 55–73). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. |
dc.relation.references | Mazur, J. E. (1994). Effects of intertrial reinforcers on self-control choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 61, 83-96. |
dc.relation.references | Mazur, J. E. (2000). Tradeoffs among delay, rate, and amount of reinforcement. Behavioural Processes, 49, 1–10. |
dc.relation.references | Mazur, J. E. (2007). Species differences between rats and pigeons in choices with probabilistic and delayed reinforcers. Behavioural Processes, 75, 220–224 |
dc.relation.references | Mazur, J. E., & Biondi, D. R. (2009). Delay-amount tradeoffs in choices by pigeons and rats: Hyperbolic versus exponential discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 91, 197–211 |
dc.relation.references | Mazur, J. E., Stellar J. R., & Waraczynski, M. (1987). Self-control choice with electrical stimulation of the brain as a reinforcer. Behavioural Processes, 15, 143-153. |
dc.relation.references | Mazur, J. E., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1988). On the functions relating delay, reinforce value, and behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 690-691. |
dc.relation.references | Mocaiber, I., Pandelis, P., Garcia, P. M., Machado, P. W., Volchan, E., Letícia de Oliveira & Vila. J. (2011). Stimulus appraisal modulates cardiac reactivity to briefly presented mutilation pictures. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 81: 299–304. DOI: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.07.014 |
dc.relation.references | Myerson, J., Green, L. & Warusawitharana, M. (2001). Area Under the Curve as a Measure of Discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 76: 235–243. |
dc.relation.references | Navarick, D. J. (1982). Negative reinforcement and choice in humans. Learning and Motivation, 13, 361-377. |
dc.relation.references | Noussair, C, N., Trautmann, S, T & Van De Kuilen, G. (2014). Higher Order Risk Attitudes, Demographics, and Financial Decisions. Review of Economic Studies, 81, 325–355. DOI:10.1093/restud/rdt032 |
dc.relation.references | Odum, A. L. (2011). Delay Discounting: I’m a K, You’re a K. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 96, 427–439. |
dc.relation.references | Odum, A. L., & Rainaud, C. P. (2003). Discounting of delayed hypothetical money, alcohol, and food. Behavioural Processes, 64, 305–313. |
dc.relation.references | Paglieri, F. (2013). The Costs of Delay: Waiting Versus Postponing in Intertemporal Choice. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 99, 362–377. |
dc.relation.references | Parker, R. I., Vannest, K. J., Davis, J. L., & Sauber, S. B. (2011). Combining Nonoverlap and Trend for Single-Case Research: Tau-U. Behavior Therapy, 42(2), 284–299. DOI:10.1016/j.beth.2010.08.006 |
dc.relation.references | Perone, M. (2003). Negative Effects of Positive Reinforcement. The Behavior Analyst, 26, 1-14. |
dc.relation.references | Petry, N. M., & Casarella, T. (1999). Excessive discounting of delayed rewards in substance abusers with gambling problems. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 56, 25–32. |
dc.relation.references | Rachlin, H. (2006). Notes on Discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 85, 425–435. |
dc.relation.references | Rachlin, H., & Green, L. (1972). Commitment, choice and self-control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 17, 15-22. |
dc.relation.references | Rachlin, H., Raineri, A., & Cross, D. (1991). Subjective probability and delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 55, 233-244. |
dc.relation.references | Raineri, A., & Rachlin, H. (1993). The effect of temporal constraints on the value of money and other commodities. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 6, 77-94. |
dc.relation.references | Rakap, S. (2015). Effect sizes as result interpretation aids in single-subject experimentalresearch: description and application of four nonoverlap methods. British Journal of Special Education, 42(1):11-33. DOI 10.1111/1467-8578.12091. |
dc.relation.references | Resolución 8430, Título II, Capítulo 1, (1993). |
dc.relation.references | Richards, J. B., Mitchell, S. H., Harriet de Wit., & Seiden, L. S. (1997). Determination of discount functions in rats with an adjusting-amount procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 67, 353–366. |
dc.relation.references | Richards, J. B., Zhang, L., Mitchell, S. H., & Harriet de Wit. (1999). Delay or probability discounting in a model of impulsive behavior: Effect of alcohol. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71, 121-143. |
dc.relation.references | Rodríguez, M. L., & Logue, A. W. (1988). Adjusting Delay to Reinforcement: Comparing Choice in Pigeons and Humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 105-117. |
dc.relation.references | Silva, J. R. (2011). International Affective Picture System (IAPS) in Chile: A crosscultural adaptation and validation study. Terapia Psicológica, 29, 251-258. |
dc.relation.references | Solnick, J. V., Kannenberg, C. H., Eckerman, D. A., & Wailer, M. B. (1980). An experimental analysis of impulsivity and impulse control in humans. Learning and Motivation, 11, 61-77. |
dc.relation.references | Sohn, K. (2019). Understanding the order effect in eliciting risk aversion. Finance Research Letters, 30, 314–317 |
dc.relation.references | Vanderveldt, A., Oliveira, L., & Green, L. (2016). Delay Discounting: Pigeon, Rat, Human—Does It Matter? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition, 42, 141–162. |
dc.rights.accessrights | info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess |
dc.subject.proposal | Self-control |
dc.subject.proposal | Auto-control |
dc.subject.proposal | Impulsividad |
dc.subject.proposal | Impulsiveness |
dc.subject.proposal | Non-contingent aversive stimulation |
dc.subject.proposal | Estimulación aversiva no contingente |
dc.subject.proposal | Time discounting |
dc.subject.proposal | Descuento temporal |
dc.subject.proposal | Choice behavior |
dc.subject.proposal | Conducta de elección |
dc.subject.proposal | Humanos |
dc.subject.proposal | Humans |
dc.type.coar | http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_1843 |
dc.type.coarversion | http://purl.org/coar/version/c_ab4af688f83e57aa |
dc.type.content | Text |
oaire.accessrights | http://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2 |