Distribución de tiempo de interacción como igualación de distancia social. Aplicación en el juego de la confianza

dc.contributor.advisorClavijo Alvarez, Álvaro Arturo
dc.contributor.authorGil Mateus, Edwin Oswaldo
dc.contributor.orcidGil-Mateus, Edwin Oswaldo [0000000277120137]spa
dc.contributor.researchgateGil-Mateus, Edwin O. []spa
dc.date.accessioned2023-08-23T14:11:14Z
dc.date.available2023-08-23T14:11:14Z
dc.date.issued2023-08
dc.descriptionilustraciones, diagramasspa
dc.description.abstractLa distancia social (DS) es una medida psicofísica sobre la cercanía o lejanía con la que una persona percibe a otras. En el análisis de la conducta esta medida ha sido usada en el descuento social, en forma de ordenación de preferencia con la posición de cada persona en un grupo definido previamente. Una vez establecida la ordenación, el participante elige en función de la distancia social entregar un monto de dinero. Si la distancia es mayor, el dinero entregado disminuye y viceversa, así que la distancia social pronostica la conducta altruista. La medición de distancia social ha indicado una preferencia ordinal, pero no muestra la magnitud o cardinalidad de la preferencia, así como advertir la relevancia de la interacción en la medida. De igual manera, la distancia social es asemejada a una dimensión física de longitud (espacio) como estructuración de las relaciones sociales. En contraste, considerando la interacción en la ordenación de preferencia, el tiempo como otra dimensión física que podría indicar una medida de distancia social. Con lo anterior, la ordenación de preferencia medida por el tiempo puede relacionarse, a su vez, con conductas prosociales en las que sean asignados recursos a otros con un costo propio para quien los asigna. Esta investigación propone que la disposición a distribuir tiempo para interactuar (TI) iguala a la ordenación de distancia social. También se plantea que la distribución de tiempo puede servir como predictor de la asignación de recursos a otros. Las hipótesis son evaluadas en cuatro estudios. En el primero, los participantes imaginaron grupos con personas conocidas, distribuyeron tiempos de interacción (duración, espera y repetición), para después ordenar el grupo de acuerdo con la escala discreta de distancia social. En el segundo, otros participantes conformaron grupos en los que interactuaron realmente en dos momentos realizando tareas académicas, desarrollando al final de cada momento la tarea de distribución de tiempos de duración de interacción y la ordenación de preferencia. Se plantearon los dos momentos para revisar el efecto de la interacción real sobre la disposición a distribuir tiempo para volver a interactuar, además de observar si la ordenación de distancia social se mantuvo o cambió. En el tercero, igual que en el primer estudio, los participantes imaginaron grupos de personas que conocían para desarrollar con la distribución de tiempo en la duración a volver a interactuar y la ordenación de preferencia, para después hacer una tarea de entrega de dinero con la estructura del juego de la confianza. En el cuarto, los participantes conformaron grupos llevando a cabo interacciones reales con labores académicas durante cuatro momentos, para al final de cada momento llevar a cabo las tareas de asignación, ordenación y entrega de dinero. Los 4 momentos fueron considerados para examinar el cambio de ajuste de las variables dada la interacción. En general, la evidencia encontrada muestra una relación inversa entre la distribución de tiempo de interacción y la ordenación de distancia social. Además, la distribución de tiempo de interacción, como igualación de distancia social, predice la conducta de entrega de dinero a otros, según lo registrado con el esquema de juego de confianza utilizado. Los resultados del estudio 1 indicaron que las medidas de distribución de tiempo como duración de una interacción y disposición a seguir interactuando, si aumentan la distancia social es menor, mientras que con la espera para volver a interactuar la relación fue directa. El estudio 2, además de coincidir las estimaciones con el anterior, mostró que las 3 medidas de asignación de tiempo mejoraron del momento 1 al 2, en especial para el individuo considerado como más cercano. En el estudio 3, la duración de la interacción explicó en 85% la variación de la DS, mientras que la duración de la interacción está correlacionada directamente con la entrega de dinero en el juego, con un R2=0,71. Finalmente, el estudio 4 con los datos obtenidos en los cuatro momentos, fue corroborado lo hallado en el anterior, con R2=0,98. En el momento 3 de este estudio, cuando los grupos se reconfiguraron con participantes sin interacción, el TI explicó la DS en 73%. (Texto tomado de la fuente)spa
dc.description.abstractSocial distance (SD) is a psychophysical measure of the closeness or distance with which a person perceives others. In behavior analysis, this measure is used in the social discount, in the form of preference ordering with the position of each person in a previously defined group. Once the ordering is established, the participant chooses based on the social distance to deliver an amount of money, so if the distance is greater, the money delivered decreases and vice versa, so the social distance predicts altruistic behavior. The measurement of social distance has indicated an ordinal preference, but does not show the magnitude or cardinality of the preference, as well as noticing the relevance of the interaction in the measurement. In the same way, social distance is similar to a physical dimension of length (space) as a structuring of social relations. In contrast, considering the interaction in preference ordering, time as another physical dimension that could indicate a measure of social distance. With the above, the ordering of preference measured by time can be related, in turn, to prosocial behaviors in which resources are assigned to others at their own cost to the assigner. This research proposes that the willingness to allocate time to interact (TI) equals the ordering of social distance. It is also suggested that the distribution of time can serve as a predictor of the allocation of resources to others. The hypotheses are tested in four studies. In the first, the participants imagined groups with familiar people, distributed interaction times (duration, wait, and repetition), and then ordered the group according to the discrete scale of social distance. In the second, other participants formed groups in which they actually interacted in two moments carrying out academic tasks, developing at the end of each moment the task of distribution of interaction duration times and the ordering of preference. The two moments were considered to review the effect of actual interaction on the willingness to allocate time to interact again, in addition to observing whether the order of social distance was maintained or changed. In the third, as in the first study, the participants imagined groups of people they knew to develop with the distribution of time in the duration to re-interact and the ordering of preference, to later do a task of giving money with the structure of the trust game. In the fourth, the participants formed groups carrying out real interactions with academic tasks during four moments, to at the end of each moment carry out the tasks of assigning, ordering and delivering money. The 4 moments were considered to examine the adjustment change of the variables given the interaction. In general, the evidence found shows an inverse relationship between the distribution of interaction time and the ordering of social distance. In addition, the distribution of interaction time, such as social distance matching, predicts the behavior of giving money to others, as recorded with the trust game scheme used. The results of study 1 indicated that the measures of time distribution such as duration of an interaction and willingness to continue interacting, if they increase the social distance is less, while with the wait to interact again the relationship was direct. Study 2, in addition to matching the estimates with the previous one, showed that the 3 measures of time allocation improved from moment 1 to moment 2, especially for the individual considered closest. In study 3, the duration of the interaction explained 85% of the variation in SD, while the duration of the interaction is directly correlated with the delivery of money in the game, with R2=0.71. Finally, study 4 with the data obtained at the four moments, corroborated what was found in the previous one, with R2=0.98. At time 3 of this study, when the groups were reconfigured with participants without interaction, the IT explained the SD in 73%.eng
dc.description.degreelevelDoctoradospa
dc.description.degreenameDoctor en Psicologíaspa
dc.description.researchareaPsicología Básica y Experimental Elecciones socialesspa
dc.format.extent121 páginasspa
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdfspa
dc.identifier.instnameUniversidad Nacional de Colombiaspa
dc.identifier.reponameRepositorio Institucional Universidad Nacional de Colombiaspa
dc.identifier.repourlhttps://repositorio.unal.edu.co/spa
dc.identifier.urihttps://repositorio.unal.edu.co/handle/unal/84588
dc.language.isospaspa
dc.publisherUniversidad Nacional de Colombiaspa
dc.publisher.branchUniversidad Nacional de Colombia - Sede Bogotáspa
dc.publisher.facultyFacultad de Ciencias Humanasspa
dc.publisher.placeBogotá, Colombiaspa
dc.publisher.programBogotá - Ciencias Humanas - Doctorado en Psicologíaspa
dc.relation.referencesAkerlof, G., (1997). Social Distance and Social Decisions. Econometrica, Vol 65, No. 5 (September), pp. 1005-1027. https://doi.org/10.2307/2171877spa
dc.relation.referencesBandura A. (1978) The Self System in Reciprocal Determinism. American Psychologist, Vol 33, No. 4, April. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.33.4.344spa
dc.relation.referencesBandura, A. (1983). Temporal dynamics and decomposition of reciprocal determinism: A reply to Phillips and Orton. Psychological Review, 90(2), 166–170. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.90.2.166spa
dc.relation.referencesBanton, M. (1960). Social Distance: A New Appreciation. The Sociological Review, 8(2), 169–183. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-954X.1960.tb01033.xspa
dc.relation.referencesBargh, J., & Williams, E. L. (2006). The automaticity of social life. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00395.xspa
dc.relation.referencesBaum, W. M. (1974). On Two Types of Deviation from the Matching Law: Bias and Undermatching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 22 (1), 231-242. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1974.22-231spa
dc.relation.referencesBaum, W. M. (1997). The trouble with time (chapter 3). https://www.academia.edu/51213107/The_trouble_with_time. In Ghezzi, P., Hayes L. J. (1997) Investigations in Behavioral Epistemology. Context Press, 239 pages.spa
dc.relation.referencesBaum, W. M. (2004). Molar and molecular views of choice. Behavioural Processes, 66(3), 349–359. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2004.03.013spa
dc.relation.referencesBaum, W. (2012) “Rethinking reinforcement: Allocation, induction and contingency”. Journal of experimental análisis of behavior, 97, number 1 (january), pp. 101-124. https://doi.org/10.1901%2Fjeab.2012.97-101spa
dc.relation.referencesBaum, W. M. (2015a) The role of induction in operant schedule performance. In Behavioural Processes 114, 26–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.01.006spa
dc.relation.referencesBaum, W. M. (2015b) Driven by Consequences: The Multiscale Molar View of Choice. Managerial. Decision. Economics. 37: 239–248. https://doi.org/10.1002/mde.2713spa
dc.relation.referencesBaum, W. (2018) Three Laws of Behavior: Allocation, Induction and Covariance. Behavior Analysis: Research and Practice, Vol 18, No. 3, pp. 239-251. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/bar0000104spa
dc.relation.referencesBaum, W. M., Rachlin, H. C. (1969). Choice as Time Allocation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 12 (6), 861-874. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1969.12-861spa
dc.relation.referencesBechler, C., Green, L., Myerson, J. (2015). Proportion offered in the Dictator and Ultimatum Games decreases with amount and social distance. Behavioural Processes, 115, 149-155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.04.003spa
dc.relation.referencesBelisle, J.; Paliliunas, D.; Vangsness, L; Dixon, M. R.; Stanley, C. R. (2020) Social Distance and Delay Exert Multiple Control over Altruistic Choices. The Psychological Record 70, pp 445–457. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-020-00399-xspa
dc.relation.referencesBen-Ami Bartal, I., Rodgers, D. A., Bernardez, M. S., Decety, J., Mason, P. (2014) Pro-social behavior in rats is modulated by social experience. eLife, 3:e01385. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.01385spa
dc.relation.referencesBenoit, Kenneth (2011) Linear Regression Models with Logarithmic Transformations. Methodology Institute London School of Economics. Disponible en: https://kenbenoit.net/assets/courses/ME104/logmodels2.pdf (consultado el 17 de junio de 2022)spa
dc.relation.referencesBerg J., Dickhaut J., McCabe K. (1995) Trust, Reciprocity, and Social History. Games and Economic Behavior, Volume 10, Issue 1, July, Pages 122-142. https://doi.org/10.1006/game.1995.1027spa
dc.relation.referencesBogardus, E.S. (1925) Measuring social distance. Journal of Applied Sociology, 9, 299–308. https://brocku.ca/MeadProject/Bogardus/Bogardus_1925c.htmlspa
dc.relation.referencesBogardus, E.S. (1933) A social distance scale. Sociology and Social Research, 17, 265–271. https://brocku.ca/MeadProject/Bogardus/Bogardus_1933.htmlspa
dc.relation.referencesBorrero, John C.; Crisolo, Stephany S.; Tu, Qiuchen; Rieland, Weston A.; Ross, Noël A., Francisco, Monica T.; Yamamoto, Kenny Y. (2007). An application of the matching law to social dynamics. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, 589–601 Number 4 (winter). https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1901/jaba.2007.589-601spa
dc.relation.referencesBrown, J., Rachlin, H. (1999). Self-control and social cooperation. Behavioural Processes, 47, 65–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(99)00054-6spa
dc.relation.referencesBuddiga, N. R.; Locey, M. L. (2021) Reciprocal Discounting: A Pilot Study. The Psychological Record 72, pages505–509. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-020-00449-4spa
dc.relation.referencesCamerer, C. F., (2003). Behavioral Game Theory. Experiments in Strategic Interaction. Russell Sage Foundation. Princeton University Press. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2003-06054-000spa
dc.relation.referencesConger, R.; Killeen, P. (1974) Use of Concurrent Operants in Small Group Research: A Demonstration. The Pacific Sociological Review, Vol. 17, No. 4, Oct., pp. 399-416. https://doi.org/10.2307/1388548spa
dc.relation.referencesCharlton, S. R., Gossett, B. D., Charlton, V. A. (2012). Effect of delay and social distance on the perceived value of social interaction. Behavioural Processes, 89, 23–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2011.10.003spa
dc.relation.referencesChatterjea, R. G.; Basu, A. (1978) The relationship between Social Distance and Levels of Conceptual Integration. The Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 104, pp. 299-300. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1978.9924074spa
dc.relation.referencesDodd, S. C. & Griffiths, K. S. (1958) The Logarithmic Relation of Social Distance and Intensity. The Journal of Social Psychology. 48:1, 91-101. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1958.9919271spa
dc.relation.referencesEthington, P.J. (1997) The intellectual construction of “social distance”: toward a recovery of Georg Simmel’s geometry. Cybergeo: European Journal of Geography, 30. Disponible en http://cybergeo.revues.org/227 (consultado noviembre 28 de 2021).spa
dc.relation.referencesFehr, E., Fischbacher, U. (2003) The nature of human altruism. Nature 425, 785–791. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature02043spa
dc.relation.referencesGoffman, E. (1966). Behavior in public places. New York: Free Press. 248 pages.spa
dc.relation.referencesGreen, L., & Myerson, J. (2004). A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 769–772. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.769spa
dc.relation.referencesHackman, J., Danvers, A., Hruschka D. J. (2015) Closeness is enough for friends, but not mates or kin: mate and kinship premiums in India and U.S. Evolution and Behavior 36, 137–145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.10.002spa
dc.relation.referencesHarris A, Young A, Hughson L, Green D, Doan SN, Hughson E, et al. (2020) Perceived relative social status and cognitive load influence acceptance of unfair offers in the Ultimatum Game. PLoS ONE 15(1): e0227717. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227717spa
dc.relation.referencesHerrnstein, R. J. (1961) Relative and Absolute Strength of Response as a Function of Frequency of Reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 4 (3), 267-272. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1961.4-267spa
dc.relation.referencesHoffman, Elizabeth; McCabe, Kevin; Smith, Vernon L. (1996) Social Distance and Other-Regarding Behavior in Dictator Games. The American Economic Review, Vol 86, No. 3, June, pp. 653-660. http://www.jstor.org/stable/2118218spa
dc.relation.referencesHoppler SS, Segerer R and Nikitin J (2022) The Six Components of Social Interactions: Actor, Partner, Relation, Activities, Context, and Evaluation. Frontiers of Psychology. 12:743074. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.743074spa
dc.relation.referencesHraba, J.; Hagendoorn, L.; Hagendoorn, R. (1989) The ethnic hierarchy in The Netherlands: Social distance and social representation. In British Journal of Social Psychology. Vol 28, pp. 57-69. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8309.1989.tb00846.xspa
dc.relation.referencesJohnston, J. M., Pennypacker, H. S., & Green, G. (2020). Strategies and tactics of behavioral research and practice (4th ed.). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. New York, NY. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/2019-48755-000spa
dc.relation.referencesJones, B., Rachlin, H. (2006). Social Discounting. Psychological Science 17 (4), 283-286. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01699.xspa
dc.relation.referencesJones, B., Rachlin, H. (2009). Delay, Probability, and Social Discounting in a Public Goods Game. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99 (1), 61-73. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2009.91-61spa
dc.relation.referencesJones, B. A. (2021) A Review of Social Discounting: The Impact of Social Distance on Altruism. The Psychological Record. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-021-00488-5spa
dc.relation.referencesKarakayali, Nedim (2017) “Social Distance”. In Turner, Bryan S. The Wiley Blackwell Encyclopedia of Social Theory. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118430873.est0353spa
dc.relation.referencesLie-Panis, J.; André, J.-B. (2022) Cooperation as a signal of time preferences. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biology Sciences. 2892021226620212266. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.2266spa
dc.relation.referencesLocey, M. L., Rachlin, H. (2015) Altruism and anonymity: A behavioral analysis. Behavioural Processes, 118, 71–75.spa
dc.relation.referencesLocey, M. L.; Safin, V.; Rachlin, H. (2013). Social Discounting and The Prisoner’s Dilemma Game. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 99 (1), 85-97. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.3spa
dc.relation.referencesLocey, M. L., Jones, B. A., & Rachlin, H. (2013). Self-control and altruism. In G. J. Madden, W. V. Dube, T. D. Hackenberg, G. P. Hanley, & K. A. Lattal (Eds.), APA handbook of behavior analysis, Vol. 1. Methods and principles (pp. 463–481). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13937-020spa
dc.relation.referencesMagee, J. C., & Smith, P. K. (2013). The social distance theory of power. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17(2), 158–186. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868312472732spa
dc.relation.referencesMazur, J.E. (1987). An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In M.L. Commons, J.E. Mazur, J.A. Nevin, & H. Rachlin (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: Vol. 5. The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value (pp. 55–73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1986-98701-003spa
dc.relation.referencesMele, V. (2017) “Social Interaction”. In Turner Bryan S. (2017) The Wiley Blackwell Encyclopedia of Social Theory. Edited by Bryan S. Turner. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118430873.est0811spa
dc.relation.referencesMorris, S. M., Vollmer T. R. (2022a) Increasing social time allocation and concomitant effects on mands, item engagement, and rigid or repetitive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis Vol. 55 (3), pp. 814-831. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.919spa
dc.relation.referencesMorris, S. M., Vollmer T. R. (2022b) The matching law provides a quantitative description of social time allocation in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis Vol. 55 (3), pp. 934-957. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.934spa
dc.relation.referencesNettle, D.; Harper, Z.; Kidson, A.; Stone, R.; Penton-Voak, I. S.; & Bateson, M. (2013). The watching eyes effect in the Dictator Game: It's not how much you give, it's being seen to give something. Evolution and Human Behavior 34(1):35-40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.08.004spa
dc.relation.referencesPapasteri CC, Sofonea A, Boldasu R, Poalelungi C, Tomescu MI, Pistol CAD, Vasilescu RI, Nedelcea C, Podina IR, Berceanu AI, Froemke RC and Carcea I (2020) Social Feedback During Sensorimotor Synchronization Changes Salivary Oxytocin and Behavioral States. Front. Psychol. 11:531046. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.531046spa
dc.relation.referencesPark, R.E. (1924) The Concept of Social Distance As Applied to the Study of Racial Attitudes and Racial Relations. Journal of Applied Sociology 8 (1924): 339-344. https://brocku.ca/MeadProject/Park/Park_1924.htmlspa
dc.relation.referencesParrillo, V.N. and Donoghue, C. (2005) Updating the Bogardus social distance studies: a new national survey. The Social Science Journal, 42 (2), 257–271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.soscij.2005.03.011spa
dc.relation.referencesPatterson, Miles L. (2016) Environment and Social Interaction. In Berger, Charles R. & Roloff, Michael E. The International Encyclopedia of Interpersonal Communication, First Edition. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118540190.wbeic100spa
dc.relation.referencesPremack, D. (1962) Reversibility of the Reinforcement Relation. Science 136, 255-257. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.136.3512.255spa
dc.relation.referencesRachlin, H. (1995) The Value of Temporal Patterns in Behavior. Current Directions in Psychological Science, Vol 4, No. 6, December, pp. 188-192. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772634spa
dc.relation.referencesRachlin, H. (2006). Notes on Discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 85 (3), 425-435. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2006.85-05spa
dc.relation.referencesRachlin, H., Jones, B. A. (2008a). Social Discounting and Delay Discounting. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 21, 29-43. https://doi.org/10.1002/bdm.567spa
dc.relation.referencesRachlin H., Jones, B. A. (2008b) Altruism among relatives and non-relatives. Behavioural Processes, 79, 120-123. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2008.06.002spa
dc.relation.referencesRomanowich, P. (2021) Sharing Personal Information is Discounted as a Function of Social Distance. The Psychological Record 72, pages 497–504. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-021-00494-7spa
dc.relation.referencesSafin, V., Arfer, K. B., Rachlin, H. (2015). Reciprocation and altruism in social cooperation. Behavioural Processes 116, 12-16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2015.04.009spa
dc.relation.referencesSafin, V., Locey, M. L., Rachlin H. (2013) Valuing rewards to others in a prisoner’s dilemma game. Behavioural Processes, 99, 145-149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2013.07.008spa
dc.relation.referencesSafin, V. & Rachlin, H. (2020) “A ratio scale for social distance”. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. Volume 114, Issue 1, July, Pages 72-86. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.614spa
dc.relation.referencesSamuelson, P. A. (1948). Consumption Theory in Terms of Revealed Preference. Economica, 15(60), 243–253. https://doi.org/10.2307/2549561spa
dc.relation.referencesScheele, D., Striepens, N., Güntürkün O., Deutschländer S., Maier W., Kendrick K. M., Hurlemann, R. (2012) Oxytocin Modulates Social Distance between Males and Females. The Journal of Neuroscience, November 14, 2012 • 32(46):16074 –16079. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2755-12.2012spa
dc.relation.referencesSegal, E.F., 1972. Induction and the provenance of operants. In: Gilbert, R.M., Millenson, J.R. (Eds.), Reinforcement: Behavioral Analyses. Academic, New York, pp. 1–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-283150-8.50006-Xspa
dc.relation.referencesSimmel, G. (1923) Soziologie, Dunker und Humboldt, Munich. Traducción en español como Simmel, G. (2014) Sociología: estudios sobre las formas de socialización. FCE, México. 727 pp. https://www.fondodeculturaeconomica.com/Ficha/9786071626455/Fspa
dc.relation.referencesSimon, J. L. (1995) Interpersonal Allocation Continuous with Intertemporal Allocation: Binding Commitments, Pledges, and Bequests. Rationality and Society, 7(4), 367–392. https://doi.org/10.1177/104346319500700402spa
dc.relation.referencesSimon, C.; Baum, W. M. (2017). Allocation of speech in conversation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 107(2), 258–278. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.249spa
dc.relation.referencesStaddon, J. E. R., Simmelhag, V. L. (1971). The "supersitition" experiment: A Reexamination of Its Implications for the Principles of Adaptive Behavior. Psychological Review, 78, (1), 3-43. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0030305spa
dc.relation.referencesStaddon, J. E. R. (1984) Social Learning Theory and the Dynamics of Interaction. Psychological Review, Vol 91, No. 4, pp. 502-507. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0033-295X.91.4.502spa
dc.relation.referencesToledo, Aldo Cristian; Ávila, Raúl (2017) Descuento social en pares de personas en diferentes posiciones sociales con respecto al individuo eligiendo. Conductual, Vol 5, No. 2, pp. 61-74. Disponible en: https://bit.ly/43Fbptr (consultado el 19 agosto de 2020).spa
dc.relation.referencesToledo A. C.; Avila, R. (2021) Nondiscounted Costs and Socially Discounted Benefits as Predictors of Cooperation in Prisoner’s Dilemma Games. The Psychological Record https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-020-00448-5spa
dc.relation.referencesTracy, K., (2012) “Language and Social Interaction”. In Donsbach, Wolfgang, The International Encyclopedia of Communication, First Edition. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781405186407.wbiecl006.pub2spa
dc.relation.referencesWark, C; Galliher J. F. (2007) Emory Bogardus and the Origins of the Social Distance Scale. In The American Sociologist. 38 (4): 383–395. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12108-007-9023-9spa
dc.relation.referencesWilliams, Joyce E. (2015) “Social Distance”. In Ritzer, George (2015) The Blackwell Encyclopedia of Sociology. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781405165518.wbeoss145.pub2spa
dc.relation.referencesYu, Rongjun; Hu, Pan, Hu, Zhang Ping (2015) “Social distance and anonymity modulate fairness consideration: An ERP study”. In Nature: Scientific Reports, 5, 13452. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep13452spa
dc.rights.accessrightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/openAccessspa
dc.rights.licenseAtribución-NoComercial-SinDerivadas 4.0 Internacionalspa
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/spa
dc.subject.ddc150 - Psicologíaspa
dc.subject.decsConducta social
dc.subject.decsSocial Behavior
dc.subject.decsDistanciamiento físico
dc.subject.decsPhysical Distancing
dc.subject.lembAltruismo
dc.subject.lembAltruism
dc.subject.proposalDistancia socialspa
dc.subject.proposalIgualaciónspa
dc.subject.proposalTiempo de interacciónspa
dc.subject.proposalDistribuciónspa
dc.subject.proposalSocial distanceeng
dc.subject.proposalMatchingeng
dc.subject.proposalInteraction timeeng
dc.subject.proposalAllocationeng
dc.titleDistribución de tiempo de interacción como igualación de distancia social. Aplicación en el juego de la confianzaspa
dc.title.translatedAllocation of interaction time as matching of social distance. Application in trust gameeng
dc.typeTrabajo de grado - Doctoradospa
dc.type.coarhttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_db06spa
dc.type.coarversionhttp://purl.org/coar/version/c_ab4af688f83e57aaspa
dc.type.contentTextspa
dc.type.driverinfo:eu-repo/semantics/doctoralThesisspa
dc.type.redcolhttp://purl.org/redcol/resource_type/TDspa
dc.type.versioninfo:eu-repo/semantics/acceptedVersionspa
dcterms.audience.professionaldevelopmentEstudiantesspa
dcterms.audience.professionaldevelopmentInvestigadoresspa
dcterms.audience.professionaldevelopmentPúblico generalspa
oaire.accessrightshttp://purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2spa

Archivos

Bloque original

Mostrando 1 - 1 de 1
Cargando...
Miniatura
Nombre:
79733008.2023.pdf
Tamaño:
1.94 MB
Formato:
Adobe Portable Document Format
Descripción:
Tesis de Doctorado en Psicología

Bloque de licencias

Mostrando 1 - 1 de 1
Cargando...
Miniatura
Nombre:
license.txt
Tamaño:
5.74 KB
Formato:
Item-specific license agreed upon to submission
Descripción: